Skip to main content
Log in

Runaway Social Selection for Displays of Partner Value and Altruism

  • Published:
Biological Theory Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Runaway social selection resulting from partner choice may have shaped aspects of human cooperation and complex sociality that are otherwise hard to account for. Social selection is the subtype of natural selection that results from the social behaviors of other individuals. Competition to be chosen as a social partner can, like competition to be chosen as a mate, result in runaway selection that shapes extreme traits. People prefer partners who display valuable resources and bestow them selectively on close partners. The resulting phenotypic covariance between displays and preferences gives fitness advantages to both, creating runaway selection that could shape a whole suite of prosocial traits including altruism, moral capacities, empathy, and theory of mind. Even though they give a net fitness benefit, traits at the endpoint of runaway social selection can have substantial deleterious effects on other traits such as viability, ability to accumulate resources, or vulnerability to mental disorders. Social selection forces arising from self-interested partner choices may be an invisible hand that shaped capacities for commitment, altruism, and other prosocial capacities of the human social brain.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Subscribe and save

Springer+ Basic
€34.99 /Month
  • Get 10 units per month
  • Download Article/Chapter or eBook
  • 1 Unit = 1 Article or 1 Chapter
  • Cancel anytime
Subscribe now

Buy Now

Price includes VAT (Germany)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alcock J (2001) The Triumph of Sociobiology. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander RD (1979) Darwinism and Human Affairs. Seattle: University of Washington Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander RD (1987) The Biology of Moral Systems. New York: Aldine de Gruyter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander RD (2005) Evolutionary selection and the nature of humanity. In: Darwinism and Philosophy (Hösle V, Illies C, eds), 301–348. Notre Dame, IN: University of Notre Dame Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alexander RD, Borgia G (1978) Group selection, altruism, and the levels of organization of life. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 9: 449–474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Amundsen T (2000) Why are female birds ornamented? Trends in Ecology and Evolution 15: 149–155.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ananth M (2005) Psychological altruism vs. biological altruism: Narrowing the gap with the Baldwin effect. Acta Biotheoretica 53: 217–239.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersson M (1994) Sexual Selection. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Armbruster WS, Antonsen L, Pélabonb C (2005) Phenotypic selection on Dalechampia blossoms: Honest signaling affects pollination success. Ecology 86: 3323–3333.

    Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod R (1984) The Evolution of Cooperation. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod R (1986) An evolutionary approach to norms. American Political Science Review 80: 1095–1111.

    Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod RM (1997) The Complexity of Cooperation: Agent-based Models of Competition and Collaboration. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod RM, Hammond RA, Grafen A (2004) Altruism via kin-selection strategies that rely on arbitrary tags with which they coevolve. Evolution 58: 1833–1838.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barkow JH (1989) Darwin, Sex, and Status: Biological Approaches to Mind and Culture. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Batson CD (1991) The Altruism Question: Toward a Social Psychological Answer. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Gintis H, Bowles S, Richerson PJ (2003) The evolution of altruistic punishment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 100: 3531–3535.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyd R, Richerson PJ (1985) Culture and the Evolutionary Process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradbury JW, Vehrencamp SL (1998) Principles of Animal Communication. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breden F, Wade MJ (1991) “Runaway” social evolution: Reinforcing selection forinbreeding andaltruism. Journal of Theoretical Biology 153: 323–337.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown SL, Brown RM (2006) Selective investment theory: Recasting the functional significance of close relationships. Psychological Inquiry 17: 1–29.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown SL, Nesse RM, Vinokur AD, Smith DM (2003) Providing social support may be more beneficial than receiving it: Results from a prospective study of mortality. Psychological Science 14: 320–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bull JJ, Rice WR (1991) Distinguishing mechanisms for the evolution of cooperation. Journal of Theoretical Biology 149: 63–74.

    Google Scholar 

  • Byrne RW, Whiten A (1988) Machiavellian Intelligence: Social Expertise and the Evolution of Intellect in Monkeys, Apes, and Humans. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Connor RC (1995) Altruism among nonrelatives: Alternatives to the “Prisoner’s Dilemma.” Trends in Ecology and Evolution 10: 84–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cosmides L (1989) The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the Wason selection task. Cognition 31: 187–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crespi BJ (2004) Vicious circles: Positive feedback in major evolutionary and ecological transitions. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 19: 627–633.

    Google Scholar 

  • Crook JH (1972) Sexual selection, dimorphism, and social organization in the primates. In: Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man (Campbell B, ed), 231–281. Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Darwin C (1871) The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex. London: John Murray.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawkins R (1976) The Selfish Gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Waal FBM, Macedo S, Ober J, Wright R (2006) Primates and Philosophers: How Morality Evolved. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dennett D (1995) Darwin’s Dangerous Idea. New York: Simon and Schuster.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond J (2002) Evolution, consequences and future of plant and animal domestication. Nature 418: 700–707.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dugatkin LA (1997) Cooperation Among Animals: An Evolutionary Perspective. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dugatkin LA (2006) The Altruism Equation: Seven Scientists Search for the Origins of Goodness. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar RIM (1996). Grooming, Gossip, and the Evolution of Language. Cambridge, MA, Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar RIM (1998) The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology 6: 178–190.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunbar RIM, Knight C, Power C, eds (1999) The Evolution of Culture: An Interdisciplinary View. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Fischbacher U (2003) The nature of human altruism. Nature 425: 785–791.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Henrich J (2003) Is strong reciprocity a maladaptation? On the evolutionary foundations of human altruism. In: Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation (Hammerstein P, ed.), 55–82. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Rockenbach B (2004) Human altruism: economic, neural, and evolutionary perspectives. Current Opinions in Neurobiology 14: 784–790.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank RH (1985) Choosing the Right Pond: Human Behavior and the Quest for Status. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank RH (1988). Passions within Reason: The Strategic Role of the Emotions. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank RH (1999) Luxury Fever: Why Money Fails to Satisfy in an Era of Excess. New York: Free Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank SA (1997) The Price equation, Fisher’s fundamental theorem, kin selection and causal analysis. Evolution 51: 1712–1729.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank SA (1998) Foundations of Social Evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frank SA (2006) Social selection. In: Evolutionary Genetics: Concepts and Case Studies (Fox CW, Wolf JB, eds), 350–363. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gibbard A (1990) Wise Choices, Apt Feelings: A Theory of Normative Judgment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H (2000) Strong reciprocity and human sociality. Journal of Theoretical Biology 206: 169–179.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H, Smith EA, Bowles S (2001) Cooperation and costly signaling. Journal of Theoretical Biology 119: 103–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grafen A (1984) Natural selection, kin selection, and group selection. In: Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach (Krebs JR, Davies NB, eds), 62–84. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grafen A (1990) Biological signals as handicaps. Journal of Theoretical Biology 144: 517–546.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton WD (1964) The genetical evolution of social behavior, I and II. Journal of Theoretical Biology 7: 1–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammerstein P (2001) Games and markets: Economic behavior in humans and other animals. In: Economics in Nature: Social Dilemmas, Mate Choice and Biological Markets (Noë R, van Hooff JARAM, Hammerstein P, eds), 1–22. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammerstein P, ed (2003) Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Dahlem University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Boyd R (2001) Why people punish defectors: Weak conformist transmission can stabilize costly enforcement of norms in cooperative dilemmas. Journal of Theoretical Biology 208: 79–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Boyd R, Bowles S, Camerer C, Fehr E, Gintis H, McElreath R, Alvard M, Barr A, Ensminger J, Henrich NS, Hill K, Gil-White F, Gurven M, Marlowe FW, Patton JQ, Tracer D (2005) “Economic man” in cross-cultural perspective: Behavioral experiments in 15 small-scale societies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 28: 795–815.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, McElreath R, Barr A, Ensminger J, Barrett C, Bolyanatz A, Cardenas JC, Gurven M, Gwako E, Henrich N, Lesorogol C, Marlowe F, Tracer D, Ziker J (2006) Costly punishment across human societies. Science 312: 1767–1770.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hirshleifer J (1999) There are many evolutionary pathways to cooperation. Journal of Bioeconomics 1: 73–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey N (1997) Varieties of altruism—and the common ground between them. Social Research 64: 199–209.

    Google Scholar 

  • Humphrey NK (1976) The social function of intellect. In: Growing Points in Ethology (Bateson PG, Hinde RA, eds), 303–318. London: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Khalil EL (2004) What is altruism? Journal of Economic Psychology 25: 97–123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katz L (2000) Evolutionary Origins of Morality: Cross-Disciplinary Perspectives. Devon: Imprint Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kitcher P (1993) The evolution of human altruism. Journal of Philosophy 90: 497–516.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kokko H, Brooks R, Jennions MD, Morley J (2003) The evolution of mate choice and mating biases. Proceedings of the Royal Society B270: 653–664.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kokko H, Jennions MD, Brooks R (2006) Unifying and testing models of sexual selection. Annual review of Ecology and Evolutionary Systematics 37: 43–66.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs DL (2000) The evolution of moral dispositions in the human species. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 907: 132–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Krebs J, Dawkins R (1984) Animal signals: Mind-reading and manipulation. In: Behavioral Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach (Krebs JR, Davies NB, eds), 380–402. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kummel M, Salant SW (2006) The economics of mutualisms: Optimal utilization of mycorrhizal mutualistic partners by plants. Ecology 87: 892–902.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laland KN, Odling-Smee J, Feldman MW (2000) Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 23: 131–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lande R, Arnold SJ (1983) The measurement of selection on correlated characters. Evolution 37: 1210–1226.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lasch C (1979) The Culture of Narcissism: American Life in an Age of Diminishing Expectations. New York: Warner Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leach HM (2003) Human domestication reconsidered. Current Anthropology 44: 349–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leary MR, Baumeister RF (2000) The nature and function of self-esteem: Sociometer theory. In: Advances in Experimental Social Psychology (Zanna MP, ed), vol. 32: 2–51. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann L, Keller L (2006) The evolution of cooperation and altruism: A general framework and a classification of models. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 19: 1365–1376.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manson JH, Navarrete CD, Silk JB, Perry S (2004) Time-matched grooming in female primates: New analyses from two species. Animal Behavior 67: 493–500.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mealey L (1995) Sociopathy. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 18: 523–599.

    Google Scholar 

  • Midgley M (1994) The Ethical Primate: Humans, Freedom, and Morality. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller G (2007) Sexual selection for moral virtues. Quarterly Review of Biology 82: 97–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller GF (2000) The Mating Mind: How Sexual Choice Shaped the Evolution of Human Nature. New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mills J, Clark MS (1994) Communal and exchange relationships: Controversies and research. In: Theoretical Frameworks for Personal Relationships (Erber R, Gilmour R, eds), 29–42. Hillsdale, NJ, Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesse RM (1990) Evolutionary explanations of emotions. Human Nature 1: 261–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesse RM (2001) Evolution and the Capacity for Commitment. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesse RM (2004) Cliff-edged fitness functions and the persistence of schizophrenia (commentary). Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27: 862–863.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nesse RM (2006) Why so many people with selfish genes are pretty nice— Except for their hatred of The Selfish Gene. In: Richard Dawkins (Grafen A, Ridley M, eds), 203–212. London: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noë R (2001) Biological markets: Partner choice as the driving force behind the evolution of mutualisms. In: Economics in Nature: Social Dilemmas, Mate Choice and Biological Markets (Noë R, van Hooff JARAM, Hammerstein P, eds), 93–118. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noë R, Hammerstein P (1994) Biological markets: Supply and demand determine the effect of partner choice in cooperation, mutualism and mating. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 35: 1–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noë R, Hammerstein P (1995) Biological markets. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 10: 336–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Noë R, van Hooff JARAM, Hammerstein P, eds (2001) Economics in Nature: Social Dilemmas, Mate Choice and Biological Markets. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowak MA (2006) Five rules for the evolution of cooperation. Science 314: 1560–1563.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nowak MA, Sigmund K (1998) Evolution of indirect reciprocity by image scoring. Nature 393: 573–577.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pepper JW (2007) Simple models of assortment through environmental feedback. Artificial Life 13: 1–9.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pepper JW, Smuts BB (2002) A mechanism for the evolution of altruism among nonkin: Positive assortment through environmental feedback. American Naturalist 160: 205–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pilot M (2005) Altruism as advertisement: A model of the evolution of cooperation based on Zahavi’s handicap principle. Ethology, Ecology and Evolution 17: 217–231.

    Google Scholar 

  • Price EO (1984) Behavioral aspects of animal domestication. Quarterly Review of Biology 59: 1–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oueller DC (1992) A general model for kin selection. Evolution 46: 376–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oueller DC, Ponte E, Bozzaro S, Strassmann JE (2003) Single-gene green-beard effects in the social amoeba Dictyostelium discoideum. Science 299: 105–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oueller DC, Strassmann JE (1998) Kin selection and social insects. BioScience 48: 165–175.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ridley M (1997) The Origins of Virtue: Human Instincts and the Evolution of Cooperation. New York: Viking.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riolo RL, Cohen MD, Axelrod R (2001) Evolution of cooperation without reciprocity. Nature 414: 441–443.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts G (1998) Competitive altruism: From reciprocity to the handicap principle. Proceedings of the Royal Society B265: 427–431.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rothstein SI (1980) Reciprocal altruism and kin selection are not clearly separable phenomena. Journal of Theoretical Biology 87: 255–261.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roughgarden J, Oishi M, Akcay E (2006) Reproductive social behavior: Cooperative games to replace sexual selection. Science 311: 965–969.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sachs JL, Mueller UG, Wilcox TP, Bull JJ (2004) The evolution of cooperation. Quarterly Review of Biology 79: 135–160.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schaller M, Crandall CS, eds (2003) The Psychological Foundations of Culture. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segerstråle UCO (2000) Defenders of the Truth: The Battle for Science in the Sociobiology Debate and Beyond. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sigmund K (1993) Games of Life: Explorations in Ecology, Evolution, and Behaviour. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silk JB (2003) Cooperation without counting: The puzzle of friendship. In: Genetic and Cultural Evolution of Cooperation (Hammerstein P, ed), 39–54. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simms EL, Taylor DL (2002) Partner choice in nitrogen-fixation mutualisms of legumes and rhizobia. Integrative and Comparative Biology 42: 369–380.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simon HA (1990) A mechanism for social selection and successful altruism. Science 250: 1665–1668.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith A (1976) The Theory of Moral Sentiments. Oxford: Clarendon Press. Orig. 1759.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smuts BB (1985) Sex and Friendship in Baboons. New York: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stevens JR, Cushman FA, Hauser MD (2005) Evolving the psychological mechanisms for cooperation. Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics 36: 499–518.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka Y (1996) Social selection and the evolution of animal signals. Evolution 50: 512–523.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tibbetts EA (2004) Complex social behaviour can select for variability in visual features: A case study in Polistes wasps. Proceedings of the Royal Society B271: 1955–1960.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello M (1999) The Cultural Origins of Human Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tooby J, Cosmides L (1996) Friendship and the Banker’s Paradox: Other Pathways to the Evolution of Adaptations for Altruism. In: Evolution of Social Behaviour Patterns in Primates and Man (Smith JM, Runciman WG, Dunbar RIM, eds), 119–143. London: British Academy/Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers RL (1971) The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology 46: 35–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers RL (1985) Social Evolution. Menlo Park: California, Benjamin/Cummings.

    Google Scholar 

  • Trivers R (2000) The elements of a scientific theory of self-deception. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 907: 114–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veblen T (1899) The Theory of the Leisure Class: An Economic Study in the Evolution of Institutions. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weber BH, Depew DJ, eds (2003) Evolution and Learning: The Baldwin Effect Reconsidered. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wedekind C, Milinski M (2000) Cooperation through image scoring in humans. Science 288: 850–852.

    Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard MJ (1975) The evolution of social behavior by kin selection. Quarterly Review of Biology 50: 1–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard MJ (1979) Sexual selection, social competition, and evolution. Proceedings of the American Philosophical Society 123: 222–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard MJ (1983) Sexual selection, social competition, and speciation. Quarterly Review of Biology 58: 155–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • West-Eberhard MJ (2003) Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • West SA, Griffin AS, Gardner A (2007) Social semantics: Altruism, cooperation, mutualism, strong reciprocity and group selection. Journal of Evolutionary Biology 20: 415–432.

    Google Scholar 

  • West SA, Pen I, Griffin AS (2002) Cooperation and competition between relatives. Science 296: 72–75.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilkinson GS (1984) Reciprocal food sharing in the vampire bat. Nature 308: 181–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams GC (1966) Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton: Princeton University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson DS, Sober E (1994) Reintroducing group selection to the human behavioral sciences. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 17: 585–607.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson EO (1975) Sociobiology. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf JB, Brodie ED III, Moore AJ (1999) Interacting phenotypes and the evolutionary process. II. Selection resulting from social interactions. American Naturalist 153: 254–266.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wright R (1994) The Moral Animal: The New Science of Evolutionary Psychology. New York: Pantheon Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wynne-Edwards VC (1962) Animal Dispersion in Relation to Social Behavior. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wu J, Axelrod R (1995) How to cope with noise in the iterated prisoner’s dilemma. Journal of Conflict Resolution 39: 183–189.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Randolph M. Nesse.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Nesse, R.M. Runaway Social Selection for Displays of Partner Value and Altruism. Biol Theory 2, 143–155 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1162/biot.2007.2.2.143

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1162/biot.2007.2.2.143

Keywords